An Article in Educology
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
54
Self-Enhancing and Self-Defeating Ego
Goals in Mathematics Lessons:
Relationships Among Task and Avoidance
Goals, Achievement, Self-Perceptions,
Anxiety, and Motivation
(A Scientific Educology)
Einar M. Skaalvik,
Norwegian University of Technology and Science
Dragvoll, Norway
Abstract
Educology is the fund of knowledge about the
educational process, which obviously occurs within and
outside of schools. Educology includes, at the least, the
fund of knowledge about past education (historical
educology), about current states of affairs in education
(scientific educology), about effective practices within
education (praxiological educology), about meanings of
terms and sentences in education (analytic philosophical
educology) and about good education (normative
philosophical educology).
This article is a work in scientific educology. It reports
on an exploration of extant relationships among four
dimensions of goal orientation in mathematics lessons (selfdefeating
ego orientation, self-enhancing ego orientation,
task orientation, and avoidance orientation) and
mathematics achievement, self-concept, self-efficacy,
anxiety, and intrinsic motivation.
Participants in the study were 295 Norwegian
elementary school students. Data were collected at two
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
55
points of time: (1) March and April of 1999, when the
students attended their fifth year in school (Time 1) and (2)
October and November of the same year, when the students
attended their sixth year (Time 2).
Within-time regression analyses revealed that goal
orientations were systematically related to achievement,
self-conceptions, anxiety and motivation and that selfdefeating
and self-enhancing ego orientation related
differently to all these variables.
Across-time analyses failed to show that goal orientation
affected subsequent anxiety, motivation, and achievement,
but it had some predictive value for subsequent self-concept
and self-efficacy. In addition, across-time analyses
indicated that achievement, self-conceptions, motivation and
anxiety have predictive value for subsequent goal
orientation.
Introduction
Recent educological research on motivation has focused
on the importance of students’achievement goals in
relation to their success in their school studies.
An achievement goal is distinguished in relation to the
purposes of the individual (Ames, 1992). According to
achievement goal theory, individuals approach achievement
tasks with qualitatively different types of goals (Jagacinski,
Hofmann & Strickland, 1996). Moreover, students’ goal
orientations are assumed to influence their classroom
behaviour (Ames & Archer, 1988; Meece, Blumenfeld &
Hoyle, 1988; Middleton & Midgley, 1997).
In this study, an examination and an analysis were
undertaken of the relationships among achievement goals,
academic achievement, academic self-perceptions, intrinsic
motivation, and anxiety in school.
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
56
Dimensions of Goal Orientation
Two goal perspectives in particular have been given
special attention in educological research literature. These
perspectives have been given various names: (1) task
orientation versus ego orientation (Duda, 1993; Nicholls,
1989), (2) learning versus performance goals (Elliott &
Dweck, 1988), and (3) mastery versus performance
orientation (Ames & Archer, 1988). These orientations will be referred to as task orientation and ego orientation in this
article.
Task orientation means that the focus of the students’
attention is on the task (Nicholls, 1983) and that, in the
mind’s eye of the students, the tasks of learning,
understanding, and developing new skills are ends in
themselves (Ames & Archer, 1988; Duda & Nicholls, 1992;
Lens, 1994; Nicholls, 1992). Task oriented students tend to
see mastery as dependent on their effort, and their
perceptions of ability are self-referenced (Duda, 1993).
Ego-oriented students are concerned with being judged
able, and their perceptions of their ability tend to be
normatively referenced. Ability is judged by comparison
with others (Ames & Archer, 1988; Duda, 1993; Nicholls,
1983, 1989), and high ability is evidenced by doing better
than others (Ames, 1992). The goal of ego-oriented
students is typically described as that of establishing the
superiority of their ability relative to that of others, to do
better than others, or to outperform others (Ames & Archer,
1988; Duda, 1993; Duda & Nicholls, 1992; Nicholls,
Cheung, Lauer & Patashnick, 1989).
However, being preoccupied with one’s self and
concerned about how one is perceived by others may lead to
different goals for different students (Skaalvik, 1997;
Skaalvik, Valås, & Sletta, 1994). Skaalvik (1997),
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
57
discriminated between self-enhancing and self-defeating ego
orientation. Self-enhancing ego orientation means that
one’s goal is to be best or to demonstrate superior ability,
which is the typical understanding of ego orientation. Selfdefeating
ego orientation, on the other hand, may result in
trying not to be poorest and to avoid looking stupid. Similar
distinctions have been made by Elliot & Harackiewicz
(1996), Middleton & Midgley (1997) and Skaalvik et al.
(1994).
Elliot & Harackiewicz and Middleton & Midgley
distinguished between performance-approach and performance-
avoidance goals, whereas Skaalvik et al. named the
two dimensions of ego orientation offensive and defensive
ego orientation. However, neither Skaalvik et al. (1994) nor
Elliot & Harackievich (1996) measured both dimensions of
ego orientation. Measuring both dimensions, Skaalvik
(1997) found that self-enhancing and self-defeating ego
orientation was factorially distinct and that they could be
differentiated from task orientation and avoidance
orientation (see also Middleton & Midgley, 1997;
Middleton, Kaplan, & Midgley, 1998).
Harackiewicz, Barron, and Elliot (1998) point out that
although some educological theorists have discussed task
and ego orientation (mastery and performance goals) as if
they were mutually exclusive, striving to outperform others
is not necessarily inconsistent with trying to attain mastery.
In support of this view, a number of correlational
studies has found task orientation and (self-enhancing) ego
orientation to be essentially uncorrelated. Some studies
even show that they are positively correlated (e.g.,
Harackiewicz, Barron, Carter, Lehto, & Elliot, 1997; for an
overview, see Harackiewicz, Barron, & Elliot, 1998).
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
58
Measuring both self-enhancing and self-defeating egoorientation,
Skaalvik (1997) found task orientation to
correlate close to zero with self-defeating ego orientation,
but to be positively correlated with self-enhancing ego
orientation. Moreover, in the Skaalvik (1997) study, the
two dimensions of ego orientation were positively, but
weakly correlated, whereas Middleton and Midgley (1997)
found a correlation of 0.56.
Relationships Among Achievement, Self-
Perceived Abilities, Anxiety, and Motivation
Although students’ goal orientations are assumed to
influence patterns of cognition, affect, and behaviour in
achievement settings (e.g., Dweck and Leggett, 1988),
studies of relationships between goal orientations and
achievement, self-perceived abilities, anxiety, and intrinsic
motivation are few and inconclusive. Achievement and
academic self-perceptions have often been presented as
outcomes of goals (Middleton, Kaplan, & Midgley, 1998).
Still, longitudinal studies are generally lacking, and the
educological research evidence provides little information
about causal relationships.
The few available educological studies show that both
task and ego orientation are either not significantly
correlated with achievement in school and with selfperceived
abilities or that the associations are weak (Ames
& Archer, 1988; Harackiewicz, et al., 1997; Nicholls, 1989;
Skaalvik, 1997).
When significant correlations are found between task
orientation and academic self-concept or self-efficacy, they
tend to be positive (Middleton & Midgley, 1997; Meece,
Blumenfeld & Hoyle, 1988; Nicholls, 1989; Nicholls,
Patashnick & Nolen, 1985; Seifert, 1995; Skaalvik, 1997).
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
59
Inconsistent relationships have been found between ego
orientation and students' academic self-concept (Ames and
Archer, 1988; Middleton & Midgley, 1997; Nicholls, 1989;
Schunk & Swartz, 1993; Seifert, 1995).
A possible explanation for the inconsistent results is that
different educologists measure both goal orientations and
self-perceptions in different ways. Moreover, ego orientation
has been measured with instruments which resemble
the definition of self-enhancing ego orientation in this
article. Skaalvik reports small negative correlations
between self-defeating ego orientation and both academic
self-concept and self-efficacy and small positive correlations
between self-enhancing ego orientation and academic selfconcept
and self-efficacy (Skaalvik, 1997; Skaalvik et al.,
1994).
Educological studies of relationships between goal orientation
and anxiety or intrinsic motivation are very few.
There is some evidence that intrinsic motivation and anxiety
in school may be related to students’ goal orientation (Duda
& Nicholls, 1992; Elliot and Harackiewicz, 1996; Middleton
& Midgley, 1997; Kaplan & Patrick, 1996; Skaalvik, 1997).
Purpose of the Study
This study replicates a cross sectional study by Skaalvik
(1997) in which the nominated dimensions of ego orientation
were tested. Moreover, the predictive value of goal
orientations for achievement, self-perceived abilities, anxiety,
and intrinsic motivation are analyzed in a cross
sectional context and compared with previous data.
Additionally, the same predictions are analyzed in a
longitudinal perspective.
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
60
Method
Participants and Procedure
The participants in this study were 295 Norwegian
elementary school students. Data were collected at two
points of time: (1) first, in March and April 1999, when the
students attended fifth grade (Time 1) and (2) in October
and November 1999, when the students attended sixth grade
(Time 2). Intact school classes were drawn from four school
districts in a large region in Norway.
Instruments
Students’ goal orientations in mathematics were measured
by four scales consisting of four items each. The
items are displayed elsewhere (see Skaalvik, 1997).
Examples of statements which constituted the items are:
In math classes it is important for me to learn something new
( task orientation);
In math classes I try to get away with doing as little as possible
( avoidance orientation);
When I am in math classes it is important for me to avoid
looking stupid ( self-defeating ego orientation); and
I always try to do better than other students in mathematics
( self-enhancing ego orientation).
Response categories were (1) false, (2) mostly false, (3)
sometimes false/sometimes true, (4) mostly true, and (5)
true. Cronbach's alpha for self-defeating ego orientation,
self-enhancing ego orientation, task orientation, and
avoidance orientation in fifth grade were 0.75, 0.63, 0.63,
and 0.74, respectively. Corresponding values in sixth grade
were 0.81, 0.76, 0.67, and 0.83.
Mathematics achievement was measured by a 49 item
mathematics test. The test had a Cronbach's alpha of 0.90 in
fifth grade and 0.92 in sixth grade.
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
61
Mathematics self-concept was defined as the generalfeeling of doing well or poorly in mathematics. It was
measured by an 11 item modified "Self Description
Questionnaire" (Marsh 1990). Motivational and emotional
items in the original scale (e.g., "I hate math") were replaced
with items measuring perceptions of doing well or poorly.
Examples of items are:
I always do well in math
I am hopeless in math
The scale displayed a Cronbach's alpha of 0.91 in both fifth
and sixth grade.
Mathematics self-efficacy was defined as expectations of
being able to solve particular types of mathematics
problems. It was measured by presenting 24 sets of
mathematics problems to the students. For each set, the
students were asked: "How certain are you that you can do
(solve) these kind of mathematics problems?" The items
were answered according to a seven-point scale ranging
from "not certain at all" (1) to "very certain" (7).
Cronbach's alpha for the scale was 0.94 and 0.95 in fifth and
sixth grade, respectively.
Mathematics anxiety was measured by a short (5 item)
version of an eight item anxiety scale focusing on the
emotionality dimension of anxiety (see Skaalvik & Rankin,
1995). Examples of items are:
I feel calm in math lessons, and
I am nervous in lessons in mathematics
Cronbach's alpha was 0.80 and 0.82 in fifth and 6th
grade, respectively. Mathematics intrinsic motivation was
defined as interest in working with or liking to work with
math. It was measured with a short (nine item) version of a
15 item intrinsic motivation scale developed by Skaalvik &
Rankin (1995). Examples of items are as follows:
Working with mathematics is fun, and
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
62
I like mathematics.
The scale had a Cronbach’s alpha of 0.92 in both fifth
and sixth grade.
Data Analysis
Firstly, separate analyses of Time 1 and Time 2 data
were conducted by means of regression analysis, letting goal
orientation predict achievement, self-perceptions, intrinsic
motivation, and anxiety. This was done to control that the
pattern of results was similar to previous results found in a
cross sectional study by Skaalvik (1997). Analyses of Time1 and Time 2 data revealed the same pattern of results.
Therefore, in order to save space, only the results based on
data from Time 2 are reported. Secondly, regression
analyses were conducted letting goal orientation measured
at Time 1 predict achievement, self-perceptions, intrinsic
motivation, and anxiety at Time 2. Lastly, regression
analysis was conducted with achievement, self-perceptions,
intrinsic motivation, and anxiety measured at Time 1 as predictor variables and goal orientation at Time 2 as
criterion variables.
Results
Correlations among the observed variables at Time 2 as
well as statistical means and standard deviations are shown
in Table 1. The relationships among the four dimensions of
goal orientation found by Skaalvik (1997) were supported.
Self-enhancing and self-defeating ego orientation showed a
low, but positive, correlation (0.30). Task orientation was
positively correlated with self-enhancing ego orientation
(0.20), whereas it was not significantly correlated with selfdefeating
ego orientation (0.04). Avoidance orientation was
positively correlated with self-defeating ego orientation
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
63
Table 1
Correlations among the Variables, Statistical Means,
and Standard Deviations (All Measures at Time 2)
Note. EGODEF = defensive ego-orientation, EGOOFF = offensive egoorientation,
AVOID = avoidance orientation, TASK = task orientation,
ASC = academic self-concept, EFF = self-efficacy for schoolwork, EST
= self-esteem, MANX = anxiety in mathematics classes, VANX =
anxiety in verbal arts classes. All correlations above .11 are statistically
significant (p < 0.05).
EGODEF EGOOFF TASK AVOID
EGODEF
EGOOFF 0.30
TASK 0.04 0.20
AVOID 0.13 0.06 -0.35
ASC - -0.24 0.24 0.23 -0.30
EFF -0.22 0.18 0.33 -0.26
ANX 0.48 0.11 -0.14 0.27
MOTIV -0.08 0.16 0.45 -0.60
ACH -0.13 0.09 0.08 -0.11
___________________________________________________
Mean 16.99 10.98 19.05 11.06
SD 5.63 3.74 3.60 3.36
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
64
Table 1 (Continued)
Correlations among the Variables, Statistical Means,
and Standard Deviations (All Measures at Time 2)
(0.13), but was not significantly correlated with selfenhancing
ego orientation (0.06). Task orientation was
negatively correlated with avoidance orientation (-0.35).
Zero order correlations between goal orientation and
other variables in the study were also in accordance with
previous findings. Both mathematics self-concept and selfefficacy
were positively associated with self-enhancing ego
ASC EFF ANX MOTIV ACH
0.62
-0.43 -0.42
0.45 0.38 -0.32
0.30 0.30 -0.27 0.16
____________________________________________________
27.66 66.89 11.40 10.67
6.79 12.81 4.35 4.27
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
65
orientation (0.24 and 0.18, respectively), whereas these
constructs were negatively related to self-defeating ego
orientation (-0.24 and -0.22, respectively). Self-concept and
self-efficacy were also positively related to task orientation
(0.23 and 0.33, respectively) and negatively related to
avoidance orientation (-0.30 and -0.26, respectively).
Moreover, anxiety was positively related to both selfdefeating
and self-enhancing ego orientation (0.48 and 0.11,
respectively) and to avoidance orientation (0.27), whereas it
was negatively related to task orientation (-0.14). The
association between anxiety and self-defeating ego
orientation was relatively strong.
Intrinsic motivation was not significantly related to selfdefeating
ego orientation (0.08) and weakly related to selfenhancing
ego orientation. However, intrinsic motivation
was relatively strongly related to task orientation (0.45) and
to avoidance orientation (-0.60). Mathematics achievement
did not relate strongly to goal orientation. Achievement was
not significantly related to task orientation or to selfenhancing
ego orientation, and was negatively, but weakly
related to self-defeating ego orientation (-0.13) and to
avoidance orientation (-0.11).
Regression analyses of cross sectional data were
conducted in order to compare the data with previous cross
sectional data. In these analyses, goal orientation was
defined as a predictor variable, predicting achievement, selfperceptions,
intrinsic motivation, and anxiety. The analyses
based on data from Time 2 are shown in Table 2. These
analyses revealed that self-defeating and self-enhancing ego
orientation were differently associated with the dependent
variables. Mathematics achievement, self-concept, and selfefficacy
were negatively predicted by self-defeating ego
orientation (beta values were -0.15, -0.31, and -0.28,
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
66
respectively) and positively predicted by self-enhancing ego
orientation (0.13, 0.33, and 0.22, respectively).
Furthermore, anxiety was positively predicted by selfdefeating
ego orientation (0.47), but not significantly
predicted by self-enhancing ego orientation (-0.02), whereas
intrinsic motivation was significantly and positively
predicted by self-enhancing ego orientation, but it was not
significantly predicted by self-defeating ego orientation
(0.16 and -0.08, respectively). Both self-efficacy and
intrinsic motivation were positively predicted by task
orientation (0.24) and negatively predicted by avoidance
orientation (-0.16, -0.52, respectively). Avoidance orientation
also predicted self-concept negatively (-0.25). These
results replicate results previously reported by Skaalvik
(1997), they and demonstrate that self-defeating and selfenhancing
ego orientation are differently associated with a
series of variables.
Table 2
Set of Beta Weights and Multiple
Regression Coefficients -Data Collected at Time 2
Predictor Dependent Variables
Variables
SC EFF ANX MOT
EGODEF 0.31*** -0.28 0.47*** -0.08
EGOENH 0.33*** 0.22*** -0.02 0.16***
TASK 0.09 0.24*** -0.10 0.24***
AVOID -0.25*** -0.16 0.18** -0.52***
R2 0.25 0.22 0.29 0.45
F 24.01 20.77 29.17 60.13
df 4/290 4/290 4/290 4/290
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
67
Note. * = p <0 .05, ** = p <0 .01,*** = p < 0.001. EGODEF = selfdefeating
ego orientation, EGOENH = self-enhancing ego orientation,
TASK = task orientation, AVOID = avoidance orientation, SC = selfconcept
in mathematics, EFF = self-efficacy for mathematics, ANX =
anxiety in mathematics classes, MOT = intrinsic motivation in
mathematics, ACH = mathematics achievement.
The next step in the data analysis was to conduct
regression analysis in a longitudinal perspective. As could
be expected, letting goal orientation at Time 1 (instead of
goal orientation measured at Time 2) predict achievement,
self-perceptions, anxiety, and motivation measured at Time2, did not change the general picture shown in Table 2.
Table 3, however, shows the results of a series of
regression analyses defining achievement, self-perceptions,
intrinsic motivation, and anxiety at Time 2 as criterion
variables and both goal orientation at Time 1 and a measure
of the relevant criterion variable at Time 1 as predictor
variables. Thus, the ability of goal orientation to predict
subsequent measures of each criterion variable was measured
over and above the stability of the criterion variable.
About 50 % of the variance in the criterion variables
could be explained by the predictor variables. However,
goal orientation added little to the predictions made by
previous measures of the criterion variables, and only two of
these predictions were statistically significant. Selfdefeating
ego orientation at Time 1 made a weak negative
prediction of self-efficacy at Time 2 (-0.14), whereas selfenhancing
ego orientation at Time 1 made a weak positiveprediction of self-concept at Time 2 (0.12). The main
conclusion, therefore, is that in a longitudinal perspective
and controlled for previous measures of mathematics
achievement, self-concept, self-efficacy, anxiety, and
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
68
intrinsic motivation, goal orientation had practically no
predictive value for these variables.
Lastly, regression analyses were conducted with goal
orientation measured at Time 2 as criterion variables.
Predictor variables were mathematics achievement, self-
Table 3
Set of Beta Weights and Multiple Regression
Coefficients - Predictor Variables Measured at Time 1
and Dependent Variables Measured at Time 2
Note. * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001. EGODEF = selfdefeating
ego orientation, EGOENH = self-enhancing ego orientation,
TASK = task orientation, AVOID = avoidance orientation,, SC = selfconcept
in mathematics, EFF = self-efficacy for mathematics, ANX =
anxiety in mathematics classes, MOT = intrinsic motivation in
mathematics, ACH = mathematics achievement.
Predictor Dependent Variables at Time 2
Variables ___________________________________________
at Time 1 SC EFF ANX MOT ACH
EGODEF -0.03 -0.14* 0.09 0.06 -0.01
EGOENH 0.12* 0.08 -0.06 -0.04 0.07
TASK 0.01 -0.02 0.06 -0.05 -0.01
AVOID -0.08 -0.03 0.06 -0.09 -0.09
SC 0.60***
EFF 0.64***
ANX 0.55***
MOT 0.69***
ACH 0.72***
R2 0.46 0.49 0.38 0.52 0.57
F 48.37 54.99 36.16 62.38 68.99
df 5/289 5/289 5/289 5/289 5/263
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
69
concept, self-efficacy, anxiety, and intrinsic motivation
measured at Time 1. In each analysis, a measure of the
relevant goal orientation at Time 1 was also included as a
predictor variable (Table 4). The stability coefficients were
somewhat lower for goal orientation than comparable
stability coefficients for self-perception, motivation,
Table 4
Set of Beta Weights and Multiple Regression
Coefficients with Goal Orientation at Time 2 as
Dependent Variables
Note. * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001. EGODEF = selfdefeating
ego orientation, EGOENH = self-enhancing ego orientation,
TASK = task orientation, AVOID = avoidance orientation, SC = selfconcept
in mathematics, EFF = self-efficacy for mathematics, ANX =
anxiety in mathematics classes, MOT = intrinsic motivation in
mathematics, ACH = mathematics achievement.
Predictor Dependent Variables at Time 2
Variables ____________________________________
at Time 1 EGODEF EGOENH TASK AVOID
ACH 0.03 -0.03 -0.12* 0.07
SC 0.10 0.10 0.07 0.10
EFF -0.01 0.14* 0.14* -0.09
MOT -0.03 0.05 0.05 -0.15*
ANX 0.20 0.22*** 0.00 0.02
EGODEF 0.44**
EGOENH 0.47***
TASK 0.42***
AVOID 0.50***
R2 0.30 0.34 0.28 0.52
F 18.74 24.11 17.83 62.38
df 6/276 6/276 6/276 5/289
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
70
achievement, and anxiety (see Table 3). Controlled for
corresponding ego orientation at Time 1, both selfenhancing
and self-defeating ego orientation at Time 2 were
positively predicted by anxiety at Time 1 (0.22 and 0.20,
respectively). In comparison, goal orientation at Time 1 did
not predict anxiety at Time 2 controlled for previous
measure of anxiety (Table 3). Both task orientation and
self-enhancing ego orientation at Time 2 were positively, butweakly predicted by self-efficacy at Time 2 (0.14).
Motivation at time one also predicted avoidance orientation
at Time 2 negatively (-0.15).
Discussion
This study confirms previous findings. As in previous
studies, it shows that in a mathematics context one may
discriminate between two relatively independent dimensions
of ego orientation. These dimensions are self-enhancing
and self-defeating ego-orientation. A common feature in
the two dimensions of ego orientation is that ego oriented
students are preoccupied with themselves. They compare
their abilities to other students, and they preoccupy
themselves with how they are perceived by other students.
Self-enhancing ego orientation is defined by the goal of
demonstrating superior abilities, whereas self-defeating ego
orientation is defined by the goal of avoiding looking stupid
or being negatively judged by others.
In accordance with previous educological research, the
two dimensions of ego orientation were weakly, but positively
correlated. Thus, there was a weak tendency that
students who were oriented towards demonstrating superior
abilities also were preoccupied with avoiding showing their
weaknesses. Moreover, both self-defeating and self-enhancing
ego orientation were weakly related to task orientation
and avoidance orientation, although task orientation and
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
71
avoidance orientation were moderately and negatively
related. As previously demonstrated, task orientation
correlated close to zero with self-defeating ego orientation,
whereas it correlated positively, but weakly with selfenhancing
ego orientation. Thus, it is important to note that
neither of the two dimensions of ego orientation predicted
task orientation negatively.
This study demonstrates that the two dimensions of ego
orientation are differently related to other variables. In fact,
they related differently to all other variables in the study.
The (cross sectional) regression analyses displayed in Table
2 show that self-enhancing ego-orientation relates positively
to self-concept, self-efficacy, and achievement, whereas
self-defeating ego-orientation relates negatively to these
constructs. Moreover, self-defeating ego orientation relates
positively and relatively strongly to anxiety, whereas selfenhancing
ego orientation does not relate significantly to
anxiety. Similarly, self-enhancing ego orientation relates
positively to intrinsic motivation, whereas self-defeating ego
orientation is not significantly related to this construct.
Goal theorists traditionally assume that ego goals have a
number of negative effects, for instance increasing anxiety
and decreasing intrinsic motivation, effort, and achievement
(see for instance Harackiewicz et al., 1997, 1998). This
assumption is not always supported in empirical studies.
For instance, Harackiewicz et al.(1997) found no negative
effects of ego goals on interest. Also, Covington (2000) in a
review of research, concludes that no clear pattern has
emerged from those studies exploring the association
between performance (ego) goals and either task persistence
or the degree of effort extended. The failure to confirm
negative relations with ego goals has likely occurred
because, initially, researchers did not distinguish between
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
72
self-enhancing and self-defeating ego goals, and most
studies have used measures of self-enhancing ego goals.
The present result showing that self-defeating and selfenhancing
ego orientation are differently related to a number
of constructs is therefore highly significant. It shows that
self-defeating ego orientation negatively predicts selfconcept,
self-efficacy, anxiety, and achievement. However,
it does not show detrimental effects of self-enhancing ego
orientation, and it even indicates positive relationships
among the dimensions of ego orientation and selfperceptions,
intrinsic motivation, and achievement.
Table 2 also shows that task orientation is positively
related to self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation. Nonetheless,
in accordance with previous research (e.g.,
Harackiewicz et al., 1997), there was no evidence that task
orientation predicted achievement. Task orientation may
still have small indirect effects on achievement through, for
instance, self-efficacy and intrinsic motivation. Avoidance
orientation is negatively related to academic self-perceptions
and intrinsic motivation.
Although cross sectional analyses reveal systematic
relations among dimensions of goal orientation and
academic self-perception, intrinsic motivation, anxiety, and
achievement, the longitudinal analyses provide little
evidence that these variables are affected by achievement
goals. When controlled for previous measures of the
criterion variables, for example, achievement and intrinsic
motivation, measures of goal orientation at Time 1 had little
predictive value for subsequent measures of the criterion
variables. That is, goal orientation has little predictive value
for self-concept, self-efficacy, anxiety, intrinsic motivation,
and achievement over and above the stability of these
constructs. Significant beta values were found only for selfInternational
Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
73
concept and self-efficacy. Self-defeating ego orientation
predicted self-efficacy negatively, whereas self-enhancing
ego orientation predicted self-concept positively. Hence,
these results provide limited support for a model in which
goals affect self-perceptions, anxiety, intrinsic motivation
and achievement, which are commonly accepted beliefs
about the effects of achievement goals.
The regression analyses shown in Table 4 are therefore
based on an alternative model where achievement, selfperceptions,
anxiety, and intrinsic motivation predict goal
orientation. The results give some support to such a general
model. When controlled for previous measures of the
relevant dimensions of goal orientation, anxiety predicted
subsequent measures of both self-defeating and selfenhancing
ego orientation, intrinsic motivation predicted
subsequent avoidance orientation, achievement predicted
subsequent task orientation, and self-efficacy predicted
subsequent measures of self-enhancing ego orientation and
task orientation. Taken together, these results indicate that
goal orientation primarily may be a consequence of
achievement, academic self-conceptions, intrinsic motivation,
and anxiety. The results provide some evidence that
goal orientation may affect academic self-conceptions.
In conclusion, this study confirms that achievement
goals are systematically related to achievement, academic
self-perceptions, intrinsic motivation, and anxiety.
However, the results do not provide much evidence that
these constructs are affected by achievement goals. The
questions often raised by researchers is how to increase task
goals and decrease ego goals in order to reduce anxiety and
increase self-concept, intrinsic motivation and achievement.
An equally important educological question seems to be
how to increase academic self-concept and intrinsic
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
74
motivation and reduce anxiety in order to foster task goals
and reduce self-defeating ego goals.
The findings in this study however, need to be confirmed
in subsequent research. In future research, it will be
important to explore systematically relationships among
goal orientation and relevant constructs at different grade
levels. Furthermore, an important task for future research
will be to examine possible third variables through which
achievement goals may be related to anxiety, intrinsic
motivation, self-conceptions, and achievement. Also, future
research ought to include social goals, both because our
understanding of social goals lags behind in general
(Covington, 2000) and because we have too little
understanding of the interplay between social goals and
other academic goals.
The results of this study imply that the distinction
between self-enhancing and self-defeating ego goals is an
important one. They further imply that both educologists
and educators should pay particular attention to selfdefeating
ego orientation. Detrimental effects of selfenhancing
ego orientation, which is often claimed in the
literature, are less evident.
References
Ames, C. (1992): “Classrooms: Goals, Structures, and Student
Motivation,” Journal of Educational Psychology, 84, 261-271.
Ames, C. & Archer, J. (1988): “Achievement Goals in the Classroom:
Students' Learning Strategies and Motivation Processes,”. Journalof Educational Psychology, 80, 260-267.
Covington, M.V. (2000): “Goal Theory, Motivation, and School
Achievement: An Integrative Review ,” Annual Review of
Psychology, 51, 171-200.
Duda, J.L. (1993): “Goals: A Social-Cognitive Approach to the Study
of Achievement Motivation in Sport,” in R.N. Singer, M. Murchey,
& L. Keith Tennant (Eds.): Handbook of Research on Sport
Psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
75
Duda, J.L. & Nicholls, J.G. (1992): “Dimensions of Achievement
Motivation in Schoolwork and Sport,” Journal of EducationalPsychology, 84, 290-299.
Dweck, C.S., & Leggett, E.L. (1988): “A Social-Cognitive Approach to
Motivation and Personality,” Psychological Review, 95, 256-273.
Elliott, E.S. & Dweck, C.S. (1988): “Goals: An Approach to
Motivation and Achievement,” Journal of Personality and Social
Psychology, 54, 5-12.
Elloit, A.J. & Harackiewicz, J.M. (1996): “Approach and Avoidance
Achievement Goals and Intrinsic Motivation: A Mediational
Analysis” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 70, 461-
475.
Harackiewicz, J.M., Barron, K.E., Carter, S.M., Lehto, A.T., & Elliot,
A.J. (1997): “Predictors and Consequences of Achievement Goals
in the College Classroom; Maintaining Interest and Making the
Grade,” Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73, 1284-
1295.
Harackiewicz, J.M., Barron, K.E., & Elliot, A.J. (1998): “Rethinking
Achievement Goals: When are They Adaptive for College Students
and Why?” Educational Psychologist, 33, 1-21.
Jagacinski, C.M., Hofmann, D.A. & Strickland, O.J. (1996, April):
Effects of Goal Orientations on Goal Setting in Task- and Ego-
Involving Conditions. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Educational Research Association in New York.
Kaplan, A. & Patrick, H. (1996, April): The Effect of Students’
Achievement Goals on Affect at School. Paper presented at the
annual meeting of the American Educational Research Association
in New York.
Lens, W. (1994): “Motivation and Learning,” in T. Husen & T.
Postlethwaite (Eds.): The International Encyclopedia of
Education (2nd edition), pp. 3936-3942. Oxford: Pergamon.
Marsh, H.W. (1990): SDQ II. Manual & Research Monograph. New
York: The Psychological Corporation, Harcourt Brace Jovanovich,
Inc.
Meece, J.L., Blumenfeld, P.C., & Hoyle, R. (1988): “Students’ Goal
Orientations and Cognitive Engagement in Classroom Activities,”
Journal of Educational Psychology, 80, 514-523.
Middleton, M.J., Kaplan, A., & Midgley, C. (1998, April):
Achievement Goal Orientation and Self-Efficacy: Different Goals,
International Journal of Educology, 2002, Vol 16, No 1
76
Different Relations. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the
American Educational Research Association in San Diego.
Middleton, M.J., & Midgley, C. (1997): “Avoiding the Demonstration
of Lack of Ability: An Under-Explored Aspect of Goal Theory,”
Journal of Educational Psychology, 89, 710-718.
Nicholls, J.G. (1983): “Conceptions of Ability and Achievement
Motivation: A Theory and its Implications for Education,” in S.G.
Paris, G.M. Olson, & H.W. Stevenson (Eds.), Learning andMotivation in the Classroom, pp. 211-237. Hillsdale, New Jersey:
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Nicholls, J.G. (1989): The Competitive Ethos and Democratic
Education. Cambridge, Massachusetss: Harvard University Press.
Nicholls, J.G. (1992): “Students as Educational Theorists,” in D.H.
Schunk & J.L. Meece (Eds.), Student Perception in the Classroom.
Hillsday, New Jersey: Erlbaum.
Nicholls, J.G., Cheung, P.C., Lauer, J., & Patashnick, M. (1989):
“Individual Differences in Academic Motivation: Perceived
Ability, Goals, Beliefs, and Values,” Learning and IndividualDifferences, 1, 63-84.
Nicholls, J.G., Patashnick, M., & Nolen, S.B. (1985): “Adolescents'
Theories of Education,” Journal of Educational Psychology, 77,
683-692.
Schunk, D.H. & Swartz, C.W. (1993): “Goals and Progress Feedback:
Effects on Self-Efficacy and Writing Achievement,” ContemporaryEducational Psychology, 18, 337-354.
Seifert, T.L. (1995). “Characteristics of Ego- and Task-Oriented
Students: A Comparison of Two Methodologies,” British Journal
of Educational Psychology, 65, 125-138.
Skaalvik, E.M. (1997): “Self-Enhancing and Self-Defeating Ego-
Orientation: Relations with Task and Avoidance Orientation,
Achievement, Self-Perceptions, and Anxiety,” Journal ofEducational Psychology.
Skaalvik, E.M. & Rankin, R.J. (1995): “A Test of the Internal/External
Frame of Reference Model at Different Levels of Math and Verbal
Self-Perception,” American Educational Research Journal, 32,
161-184.
Skaalvik, E.M., Valås, H., & Sletta, O. (1994): “Task Involvement and
Ego Involvement: Relations with Academic Achievement,
Academic Self-Concept and Self-Esteem,” Scandinavian Journal
of Educational Research, 38, 231-243.
|
|